Living in Europe you’ll meet plenty of English learners ill-adopting certain informal speech patterns from fluent speakers. This leads to a native speaker like myself to hear these funny dialectical differences used ungrammatically, which leads me to wonder: If they can be used ungrammatically, what is the underlying grammar?? Today, I wanna look at the following family of contractions:
Going to -> Gonna
Got to -> Gotta
Want to -> Wanna
Need to -> Needa
Have to -> Hafta
At first glance one might assume that these are the results of lazy speech,
that the pattern of verb + to
prompts speakers to glob those sounds
together into one word. However, if this were truly the case, we’d expect
this contraction to appear wherever we find verb + to
. Interestingly
enough, this is not what we find.
Let’s take the following sentences for example:
I'm going to eat this sandwich.
I'm going to my house.
Only the first sentence still rings grammatical after the contraction:
I'm gonna eat this sandwich.
I'm gonna my house.
Why is this? What is the underlying logic behind words like gonna
, gotta
,
wanna
, etc.?
You’re gonna what your house?
Speakers of English who use the gonna
contraction may feel that their ears
seem to expect a verb after gonna
. In fact, attaching a verb right after
gonna
forms a grammatical sentence:
I'm gonna (sell) my house.
The reason this works is because gonna
and going to
are actually
different. While going to
is simply a verb followed by a preposition,
gonna
is a modal verb and indicates that the following verb is yet to happen.
They represent different things and thus serve different grammatical functions.
Modal and semi-modal verbs
I’ll try and be as brief as I can in explaining but if you’re interested you can read more about them here and here
Basically, modal verbs are verbs that indicates the modality of the
following verb, some examples are could
, would
, will
, should
, must
,
etc.
On the other hand semi-modal verbs are standard verbs followed by to
that
are act like a modal verb (and they’re usually a little more flexible in terms
of tense). Examples of these include: ought to
, want to
, have got to
, etc.
If all of this is true, we might expect other semi-modal verbs to be contracted into modal verbs, and in fact we do:
You need to stop
You needa stop
While if need
is anything other than a semi-modal verb the contraction fails:
There's a need to stop
There's a needa stop
Why is this Happening?
The reason for this is likely due to a process called grammaticalization. Which, to spare us again from the rigorous details that you can find here, is a linguistic phenomenon in which words that represent objects, actions, descriptions, etc. start to serve grammatical functions.
A very relevant example of grammaticalization is the transformation from noun to
modal verb that was undertaken by will
. Our word will
is almost entirely
used as a future tense marker (I will eat
, for example), but it can also
sometimes be a verb, such as in I am willing to try
. In Old English, this word
began as a noun (like in where there's a will, there's a way
) slowly
transforming into a verb synonymous with our Modern English to want
or to wish
which finally grammaticalized into our modern day modal auxiliary:
I have a will to eat Noun
I will to eat Verb
I will eat Auxiliary
If your interested, you can read more about how will
developed here
Any of this sound familiar? While the verb to go
still does represent the tangible act of a subject
moving towards something, it began to be used semi-modally. Thus, like will
,
this use of the word is diverging from its original usage and is turning into a
a grammatical marker, and in this case, diverging it’s pronunciation with it.
He s’gotta sell his house
Inspired by this video on the
potential paths that the English language may take in the future, I thought it
would be cool to try and extrapolate this change to catch a glimpse of a
potential future English. It seems to me that gonna
, wanna
, and gotta
are
the top contenders in becoming standard modal auxiliaries:
Something very interesting here, gonna
is already irregular. Speakers using the gonna
contraction sometimes say Imma
or Immuna
when speaking in the first person:
I mma/munna
You gon/gonna
He/she/it gon/gonna
We gon/gonna
You/Y'all gon/gonna
They gon/gonna
It is also not too speculative to predict that gotta
may interact with
have got to
and end up sharing its irregularity
I v'gotta
You v'gotta
He/she/it s'gotta
We v'gotta
You/Y'all v'gotta
They v'gotta
This is what it might look like:
They gonna leave soon
He s'gotta pay us
Who wanna run the field
This is the part of modern linguistics I find most fascinating, the process of viewing language not as an object meant to be spoken correctly or incorrectly, but instead as an ever-evolving organism that can be studied and classified. Whenever a variation in speech arises, there’s almost always a way to describe it in terms more nuanced than a dismissive “lazy speech”. As we’ve seen, these shifts in language are not errors per se, but manifestations of an organic process. These variations do indeed have their own internal grammar rules.
Language is constantly evolving as people subconsciously adapt to the speech around them, shaping and reshaping how we communicate over time. Seeing it in this way makes language evolution resemble a massive game of telephone. While this game inevitably distorts meaning, languages always maintain a hidden grammatical structure, one that, with careful research, can be described and understood.